GET THE APP

Self-reported Attributes Associated with Cognition and Motivation as a Function of Gender

Clinical and Experimental Psychology

ISSN - 2471-2701

Research Article - (2017) Volume 3, Issue 1

Self-reported Attributes Associated with Cognition and Motivation as a Function of Gender

Trevor Archer1*, Ann-Christine Arntén1, Sweden2 and Bengt Jansson1,2
1Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
2Job Match Talent, Job Match Sweden AB, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
*Corresponding Author: Trevor Archer, Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden, Tel: +46 31 786 42 72 Email:

Abstract

Male (n=4141) and Female (n=2648) participants in executive leadership recruitment self-report were compared with regard to their expressions of Cognition and Motivation levels derived from scores on the attributes, Focus on planning and Deliberation, and, Self-motivation and Winning instinct, respectively in each case. Although the male participants scored higher for both Cognition and Motivation parameters, the vast majority of attributes predicting or counter-predicting Cognition and Motivation, respectively, were similar among the male and female participants, i.e., gender did not separate these attributes. Self-motivation, self-control and focus on detail were predictive for cognition among women whereas mood stability and will-power were expressed among men; openness was counter-predictive for women and communicability for men. Among women, resilience and risk-taking were predictive for motivation whereas focus on details, development motivation and need for speed were expressed by men; openness was counter-predictive for women. Amongst the most predictive attributes for cognition, tolerant attitude was counterpredictive for women whereas risk-taking was expressed for men; for motivation, openness was counter-predictive for women whereas communicability was expressed for men. The findings are discussed from social cognitive organizational trends that permeate current notions of what may constitute an effective leadership “toolbox”.

Keywords: Leadership inventory; Recruitment; Cognition; Motivation

Introduction

Personal attributes affect and are affected by expectations associated with performance levels emerging from positive or negative consequences of information received by individuals in leadership positions or under fostering for leadership positions [1,2] among considerations of personal attributes for leadership positions, gender and distinctive ethnicity exert influences comparable to psychological trait [3]. Despite female medical graduates comprising more than 40% of those successful, the composition of directorships of medical board presents a markedly lower percentage of women [4], with similar trends among other academic medical associations [5], in spite of attempts to understand gender-related attitudes [6]. Nevertheless, both public and corporate administrations require the benefits of attribute-multiplicity that female leadership may endower [7], which implies that obstacles hindering female leadership recruitment as well as those reducing motivation to apply need to be addressed [8-10]. Despite efforts to the contrary, there persists leadership situations in which women in leading positions, albeit underrepresented, report that they have been disadvantaged as a result of their gender [11]. It seems hardly a secret that there still remains some degree of inertia, for a multitude of reasons; among female applicants seeking or assuming executive leadership positions since various barriers for eventual recruitment appear to be a hindrance, such as real or apparent discriminatory tendencies, parenthood or lack of interest in leadership [12-15].

Individual leader recruitment remains dependent upon a plethora of instruments, invariably utilizing personality and personal profiles, for the assessment of attributes/traits to identify decision-making, coping skills, stress-tolerance, communicative skills and numerous qualities that may contribute to effective executive performance within those concerns [16]. Originally, the Job Match Talent (JMT) recruitment test instrument was developed by Olsen [17], in collaboration with recruitment consultants, to provide a suitably designed and adapted tool for selecting competent and experienced individuals for the particular positions sought by corporate and governmental agencies for the purposes of assuming a variety of leadership appointments. To this end, the Job Match Talent test estimates individuals’ attributes that are associated with occupational leadership roles in relation to type of enterprise, level of leadership and the age-at-recruitment of applicants. In a large exploratory report, consisting of a data-base derived from over 6700 recruitment participants seeking leadership positions, the psychometric measurement of personality-related attributes of individuals who had applied for executive positions displayed marked and consistent relationships between the participants’ age-group and their self-reported personal profiles [17]. It was shown that at the higher age levels the candidates for executive leadership positions expressed a lesser interest and focus upon the specific aspects of the tasks associated with ‘job-performance’ and lesser orientation towards their own personal ambitions and goals; they expressed rather greater concern for the development of their staff and teams and shaping relationships within the teams within which they functioned.

Gender, levels of education and degree of experience (years) failed to influence significantly those strengths presented in leadership profiles whereas leaders were more likely to express “achiever-strength” than non-leaders. Consequently, it comes as no surprise that it is both organizational and personal factors that emerge as gender-related challenges against women attempting to procure leader status [9]. As indicated previously [18], organizational obstacles have been shown to occur through gender-stereotyping and preexisting “good-old-boys” networks whereas personal influences included a paucity of leadership aspirations (see above), often arising from an insufficiency of role models, family and parenthood and a surfeit of workplace mentors. A ‘transformational leadership’ with greater interpersonal-orientation was more likely to be endorsed be female applicants to executive posts [19-21]. Thus, in an industrial setting, female-dominated industries exhibited greater interpersonal-orientation than the male-dominated industries, with the former reporting more pressure from their positions [22]. The influence of gender upon leadership styles, as well as stress and mental health, seems essential to understanding leadership efficacy not least with regard to workplace resilience [23]. In this regard, the question arises as to whether or not leadership development initiatives promote leadership strengths for both genders or whether leadership strengths vary with position/culture in the context of: (i) equal access to resources and opportunities, (ii) minimizing unconscious gender bias, (iii) enhancing work-life balance, and (iv) promoting leadership engagement [24]. In view of the paucity of women in senior veterinary positions, Castro and Armitage-Chan [25] examined the influences of gender, self-esteem and year-of-study upon these tendencies. They observed that career aspiration and leadership ambition were modulated by gender with greater numbers of males students than female students presenting career aspirations which were influenced positively by selfesteem, self-confidence and experience of previous leadership in clubs/ societies all of which more apparent in the male students.

The purpose of the present study was (i) to determine whether or not the self-reported personal attributes of male and female applicants were at variance with each other, and (ii) to explore whether or not there was any difference in which attributes were predictive of “cognition” and “motivation” by male and female applicants for executive leadership positions. Expressed differences arising from self-reported attributes from male and female applicants do not necessarily reflect a greater or lesser degree of suitability/appropriateness for leadership roles since effectiveness is determined by a multitude of encroaching situational and cultural pressures. Thus, attributes predicting “cognition” and “motivation” by male and female applicants, respectively, may uncover ingredients of the leadership ‘tool-box’ as a property of gender.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A general grouping of executive leaders (N=6789) participated. The mean age of the leaders was 44 years (SD=9.3). There were 39% women and 61% men included in the study.

Instrument

Personal attitudes to and experiences of job relations and characteristics were measured with the occupational recruitment Job Match Talent (JMT) inventory. The JMT test is based on ten main scales, each presenting three subscales, and has been shown to possess a high level of congruence with health- and personality-related instruments [26]. For a detailed description of the scales, see [17].

Statistical procedure

Initially, analyses were done by use of analysis of variance (ANOVA) and co-variance (ANCOVA). Dependent variables were ‘Cognition’ (index for a1: Focus on planning and d1: Deliberation) and ‘Motivation’ (index for b1: Self-motivation abd f1: Winning instinct), respectively. Independent variable was gender, and age of leaders was used as a covariate. Conditions of the ANCOVAs were controlled for (independence between main effect and covariate; parallel slopes of lines). Moreover, the indices Cognition and Motivation were regressed on the JMT subscales over gender, respectively.

Results

It was shown that male participants expressed higher levels of both Cognition (F(1,6787)=30.2, p<0.001). and Motivation (F(1,6787)=205.0, p<0.001), as defined by scoring to the attributes, planning and deliberation, and self-motivation and winning instinct, respectively, than the female participants The effects of gender were still significant (F(1,6786)=30.5, p<0.001 for Cognition; F(1,6786)=217.3, p<0.001 for Motivation) if the co-variate age of leaders (p<0.001) was included in ANCOVAs. The conditions for the ANCOVAs were fulfilled (Figure 1).

clinical-experimental-psychology-cognition

Figure 1: Least-squares means (LS-means) for cognition and motivation as expressed by male and female applicants for leadership positions (N=6789; Males=4141, Females=2648).

Cognition

Focus on planning and deliberation attributes; motivation; selfmotivation and winning instinct attributes.

Regression analysis

Regression analysis with Cognition as the dependent variable: F(17, 2640)=122.9, p<0.0001, adj. R2=0.438, and revealed that the following attributes were predictive in the following orders of significance: For female participants: Focus on order, Diplomacy, Sphere of influence, Vision, Self-motivation, Development motivation, Persistence, Physical activity, Self-control and Focus on detail whereas the following attributes were counter-predictive: Need for speed, Power of initiative, Optimism, Risk-taking, Tolerant attitude, Displayed consideration and Openness; For the male participants, F(19,4111)=190.3, p<0.0001, adj. R2=0.466, the following were predictive: Focus on order, Diplomacy, Sphere of influence, Vision, Will-power, Development motivation, Persistence, Physical activity, and Mood stability whereas the following attributes were counter-predictive: Need for speed, Power of initiative, Optimism, Risk-taking, Tolerant attitude, and communicability. Note, the bold style text indicates those attributes that differed between male and female participants. The pattern emerging from this analysis indicates that most of the predictive attribute were shared by female and male participants. However, self-motivation, Self-control and Focus on detail were predictive for the former but not the latter, and Will power and Mood stability were predictive for the latter but not the former. Counter-predictive attributes indicated that Openness was counterpredictive for women and Communicability for men (Table 1).

Female participants Male participants
Scale B SE Beta p Scale B SE Beta p
Need for speed -0.284 0.028 -0.269 0 Need for speed -0.297 0.02 -0.281 0
Focus on order 0.156 0.014 0.19 0 Focus on order 0.183 0.011 0.221 0
Power of initiative -0.17 0.021 -0.175 0 Optimism -0.17 0.015 -0.207 0
Optimism -0.142 0.018 -0.171 0 Risk-taking -0.197 0.015 -0.202 0
Tolerant attitude -0.134 0.02 -0.126 0 Power of initiative -0.148 0.017 -0.152 0
Risk taking -0.127 0.019 -0.124 0 Vision 0.141 0.02 0.137 0
Diplomacy 0.113 0.019 0.112 0 Tolerant attitude -0.111 0.015 -0.107 0
Sphere of influence 0.121 0.02 0.111 0 Sphere of influence 0.1 0.015 0.102 0
Vision 0.115 0.024 0.109 0 Development motivation 0.143 0.02 0.086 0
Displayed consideration -0.1 0.016 -0.104 0 Displayed consideration -0.079 0.013 -0.085 0
Self-motivation 0.123 0.026 0.1 0 Physical activity 0.127 0.021 0.08 0
Development motivation 0.121 0.027 0.068 0 Persistence 0.068 0.012 0.068 0
Persistence 0.058 0.017 0.057 0.001 Willpower 0.065 0.02 0.063 0.001
Physical activity 0.084 0.026 0.054 0.001 Diplomacy 0.062 0.015 0.062 0
Self-control 0.046 0.02 0.053 0.019 Mood stability 0.057 0.013 0.061 0
Openness -0.048 0.017 -0.047 0.006 Winning instinct 0.055 0.018 0.055 0.003
Focus on details 0.03 0.015 0.036 0.042 Openness -0.051 0.015 -0.053 0.001
          Communicativity -0.038 0.017 -0.041 0.024

Table 1a: Cognition.

Female participants Male participants
Scale B SE Beta p Scale B SE Beta p
Vision 0.347 0.016 0.378 0 Vision 0.29 0.013 0.331 0
Power of initiative 0.194 0.015 0.23 0 Power of initiative 0.207 0.013 0.249 0
Willpower 0.192 0.017 0.21 0 Willpower 0.21 0.013 0.239 0
Mental energy 0.248 0.025 0.182 0 Communicativity -0.137 0.012 -0.176 0
Optimism -0.126 0.012 -0.175 0 Mental energy 0.16 0.021 0.124 0
Communicativity -0.103 0.015 -0.132 0 Physical activity 0.149 0.015 0.111 0
Impact 0.097 0.02 0.107 0 Optimism -0.077 0.01 -0.111 0
Physical activity 0.142 0.017 0.106 0 Mood stability 0.088 0.01 0.11 0
Persistence 0.081 0.012 0.091 0 Persistence 0.089 0.009 0.105 0
Focus on order 0.056 0.01 0.079 0 Impact 0.074 0.016 0.087 0
Mood stability 0.058 0.013 0.076 0 Focus on order 0.05 0.008 0.072 0
Trust in others -0.052 0.012 -0.059 0 Trust in others -0.054 0.009 -0.066 0
Sphere of influence 0.052 0.014 0.056 0 Need for speed 0.055 0.015 0.061 0
Focus on planning 0.043 0.012 0.051 0 Displayed consideration -0.038 0.011 -0.048 0.001
Openness -0.043 0.013 -0.049 0.001 Focus on planning 0.034 0.01 0.042 0.001
Concurring image 0.04 0.013 0.044 0.003 Concurring image 0.034 0.014 0.038 0.02
Resilience 0.035 0.014 0.041 0.011 Concentration 0.032 0.01 0.035 0.001
Risk taking 0.035 0.014 0.039 0.015 Focus on details 0.021 0.008 0.031 0.012
Concentration 0.031 0.012 0.034 0.007 Sphere of influence 0.026 0.01 0.031 0.013
          Development motivation 0.032 0.014 0.022 0.028

Table 1b: Motivation.

Table 1: Stepwise regression coefficients over gender, male and female participants, for the dependent variables, cognition (1a) and motivation (1b), respectively, on the JMT subscales; coefficients are sorted by beta-values as well as B, SE and p-values, respectively (N=6789). The predictor variables, for cognition (1a), need for speed, focus on order, etc., and motivation (1b) vision, power of initiative, etc., are listed below with beta values and significance level.

Regression analysis with motivation as the dependent variable: F(19,2638)=234.1, p<0.0001, adj. R2=0.625, revealed that the following attributes were predictive for both female and male participants: Vision, Power of initiative, Will-power, Mental energy, Impact, Physical activity, Persistence, Focus on order, Mood stability, Sphere of influence, Focus on planning, Concurring image, and concentration whereas the following were shared counter-predictive attributes: Optimism, Communicability and Trust in others. The following attributes were predictive for women’s Motivation only: Resilience and Risk-taking whereas for men, F(20,4110)=335.6, p<0.0001, adj. R2=0.618, it was Focus on detail, Development motivation and Need for speed. Openness was counter-predictive for motivation among women.

Analysis of the five highest Beta values for male and female participants gave differing patterns over gender for Cognition and Motivation, as follows:

Cognition: Tolerant attitude was counter-predictive for females but Risk-taking for males.

Motivation: Optimism was counter-predictive for females but Communicability for males (Table 2).

Scale SE Beta p
Females      
1. E3: Need for speed 0.028 - 0.269 <0.0001
2. A3: Focus on order 0.014   0.190 <0.0001
3. G2: Power of initiative 0.021 - 0.175 <0.0001
4. B2: Optimism 0.018 - 0.171 <0.0001
5. H2: Tolerant attitude 0.020 - 0.126 <0.0001
Males      
E3: Need for speed 0.020 - 0.281 <0.0001
A3: Focus on order 0.011  0.221 <0.0001
B2: Optimism 0.015 - 0.207 <0.0001
G3: Risk-taking 0.015 - 0.202 <0.0001
z 0.017 - 0.152 <0.0001

Table 2a: Beta values and significance levels for attributes predicting cognition.

Scale SE Beta p
Females      
F2: Vision 0.016   0.378  
G2: Power of initiative 0.015   0.230  
D2: Willpower 0.017   0.210  
E2: Mental energy 0.025   0.182  
B2: Optimism 0.012 -0.175  
Males      
F2. Vision 0.016   0.331  
G2: Power of initiative 0.015   0.249  
D2: Willpower 0.017   0.239  
J2: Communicability 0.025 -0.176  
E2: Mental energy 0.012   0.124  

Table 2b: Beta values and significance levels for attributes predicting motivation.

Table 2: Stepwise regression coefficients over gender for cognition (Table 2a) and Motivation (Table 2b), respectively, as the dependent variables and the five most predictive attributes on the JMT subscales in each case as the predictor variables; coefficients are sorted by beta-values (N=6789).

Discussion

The present findings pertain to the apparent or real differences in self-reported leadership gender attributes in relation to characteristics associated with (a) cognition and (b) motivation, between male and female applicants for executive appointments. The results may be summarized as follows: (i) The direct comparison of male and female participants indicated that the male participants reported higher levels of both cognition and motivation (Figure 1). Does this result imply that the males expressed greater cognitive capacity and a higher level of motivation than the females? In order to examine the veracity of this comparison regression analyses were performed to ascertain which attributes were associated with cognition and motivation among male and female participants respectively. (ii) The vast majority of attributes predicting or counter-predicting Cognition and Motivation, respectively, were similar among the male and female participants, i.e., gender did not separate these attributes (Table 1). (iii) Self-motivation, self-control and focus on detail were predictive for cognition among women whereas mood stability and will-power were expressed among men; openness was counter-predictive for women and communicability for men. (iv) Among women, resilience and risk-taking were predictive for motivation whereas focus on details, development motivation and need for speed were expressed by men; openness was counterpredictive for women. (v) Amongst the most predictive attributes for cognition, tolerant attitude was counter-predictive for women whereas risk-taking was expressed for men; for motivation, openness was counter-predictive for women whereas communicability was expressed for men. These patterns of predictability for Cognition and Motivation, respectively, would seem to imply that the female and male recruitment participants were each possessed of “leadership toolboxes” which we have termed “Cognition” and “Motivation”.

The present results suggest that the tentative ‘label’ “tools” or attributes/ingredients provided in each “toolbox”, i.e., whether Cognition “toolbox” or a Motivation “toolbox”, remain essentially the same, with the exceptions noted above. The finding that the Cognition and Motivation “toolboxes” for women was ‘smaller’ than that of the men does not imply a higher leadership prowess by the latter but rather that each possess slightly differing ‘tools’ for employment since it is the application of those leadership tools that remains the major issue. It was shown previously that “Willingness-to-take-risks” was observed to be the single attribute upon which female and male applicants differ with the latter expressing a greater extent [18]. None of the other attributes studied, including “humor-equilibrium”, “resilience”, “will-power”, “stamina”, “initiative”, “assent-image” and “openness” differed between the genders. Taking into account previous, traditional notions regarding the differential capacity of male and female leaders to withstand ‘stress-and-strain’, those findings demonstrated that the concomitant examination participants’ variations due to age effects imply that leadership-strength attributes are modulated by age rather than gender per se. The effect of gender on cognition and motivation was controlled for by use of the covariate age of leaders. Although age had a significant effect for both cognition and motivation, the impact of gender was not influenced. This result was different compared to the reduced gender effects that were found for eight JMT scales in a previous study [18].

The organizational context in social cognition underlying leadership perceptions has not received much attention despite the plethora in institutional reports that touch upon associated issues [27,28]. Özalp et al. [29] examined a range of subordinates’ perceptions of individuals in managerial positions as leaders (i.e., to the extent they may or may not perceive their managers as a leaders) as a potential mediating factor to explain the relationship between managers’ self-monitoring and their subordinates’ attitudes toward their organizations among middlelevel managers and their subordinates taken from different business organizations in Turkey. They observed that subordinates ‘leadership perceptions of their managers mediated the relationship between managers’ self-monitoring and their subordinates’ affective and normative organizational commitments implying that the managerial “toolbox” is required to present both variance and adaptability to context. In this regard, the importance of developmentally sequencing leadership-learning experiences and addressing evolving complexities of leadership identity reinforces the observed necessity of a wide of attributes to buttress leadership style [30], in the present case involving the cognition and motivation leadership “toolboxes”. Finally, there is an ever-increasing consensus for the requirement of a leadership of sensemaking, through the fermenting of, for example the “toolbox” type of notion, that implements front-line staff managers to exercise their collective discretionary power in strengthening [31].

Limitations

In order to extract a deeper understanding of the real or apparent nature of posited gender influences, it is suggested, from the influence of resilience and risk-taking which were predictive for women’s motivation, that coming investigations ought to be devoted to the recruitment utility propensity of the JMT instrument through coadministration with the Workplace Resilience Instrument which optimizes factors involving “active problem-solving”, “team efficacy”, confident “sense-making” and “bricolage” [23].

References

Citation: Archer T, Arntén AC, Olsen K, Jansson B (2016) Self-reported Attributes Associated with Cognition and Motivation as a Function of Gender. Clin Exp Psychol 3: 146.

Copyright: © 2016 Archer T, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.